Monday, 06 January 2025

Main

Information
Last updated: 04 January 2025 Print

Speech and Language developmental disorders

Information
Language ComprehensionLanguage RecognitionLanguage, Neural basis ofSpecific Language ImpairmentSpeech MilestonesSpeech and Language Disorders

Definition and Components of Language and Speech

  • Language: A communication system acquired through use, involving symbols sequenced to share attention and ideas.
  • Speech: One mechanism for expressing language, but other methods like gestures, sign language, and iconic symbols can also be used.

Pre-linguistic Skills and Joint Attention

  • Joint attention, the ability to share focus with another, is crucial for language acquisition.

Disorders Affecting Speech and Language Acquisition

  • Defined by inclusionary (specific deficits) and exclusionary (absence of other explanations) criteria.
  • Common conditions disrupt the typical progression of speech and language in children, measured relative to peers.

Diagnostic Criteria

  • ICD-10 (WHO) and DSM-5 (APA) provide classifications:
    • ICD-10 distinguishes receptive, expressive, and mixed disorders.
    • DSM-5 groups these under "communication disorders" with subcategories:
      • Language disorder
      • Speech sound disorder
      • Childhood-onset fluency disorder (stuttering)
      • Social (pragmatic) communication disorder
      • Unspecified communication disorder

Speech Disorders

  • Speech sound disorder: Persistent difficulty with speech sound production affecting intelligibility.
  • Dysfluency (e.g., stuttering):
    • Repetition of sounds, syllables, or words; silent blocks; and associated physical movements.
    • Often resolves between ages 3-5 but can become chronic with risk factors like family history.

Pragmatic Language Impairment

  • Affects everyday language use without the social and behavioral features of ASD.

Severe and Rare Conditions

  • Childhood Apraxia of Speech (CAS)/Verbal Dyspraxia: Impaired motor planning for speech, debated underlying mechanisms.
  • Dysarthria: Weakness or involuntary movements affecting articulation, often in acquired disorders.
  • Phonation Issues: Problems with voice projection and breath control, worsening intelligibility.

Neurological Paroxysmal Disorders

  • DSM-5 includes speech and language disorders linked to epilepsy, affecting development and acquired abilities.
  • Certain anticonvulsants may exacerbate speech issues.

Prevalence and Demographics

  1. Late Talkers (2-Year-Olds):

    • Approximately 15% of 2-year-olds exhibit delayed speech, termed "late talking toddlers."
    • Causes are multifactorial and include:
      • Family history of delayed language acquisition.
      • Socioeconomic factors, such as poverty and low maternal education.
    • Male sex, family history, and preterm birth are identified as risk factors.
  2. Speech Delay in Early School Age (6-Year-Olds):

    • Overall prevalence: 3.8%.
    • Gender difference: More prevalent in boys (4.5%) than girls (3.1%).
    • Speech delay and language impairment comorbidity:
      • 1.3% overall.
      • 0.51% when the language impairment is specific (normal nonverbal intellect).
    • Persistent Specific Language Impairment (SLI):
      • 5-8% of children with SLI also experience speech delay.
  3. Primary Specific Speech and Language Disorders (5–6-Year-Olds):

    • Prevalence is 7.4% overall (8% boys, 6% girls) based on a composite language score of below 1.25 SD.
    • Parental education is associated, with lower levels linked to higher prevalence.

Risk and Long-Term Implications

  • Persistent phonological difficulties (e.g., deletion/substitution errors) can lead to:

    • Reading and spelling challenges.
    • Academic struggles.
    • Social acceptance issues in school.
  • Intellectual impairment is associated with persistent speech and language delays in kindergarten:

    • Affects 50% of girls.
    • Affects 20% of boys.

Screening and Identification

  • No evidence supports targeted screening for speech and language delay to improve outcomes.
  • Most cases are identified through clinical surveillance or presentation.

Speech Production and Phonological System

  • Speech relies on a phonemic system:

    • Phonological Component: Rules for sound contrast and structure.
    • Articulatory Component: Sensory-motor movements of speech organs.
    • Praxic Component: Planning and programming spatiotemporal movements for speech.
    • Prosodic Component: Stress, intonation, and voice quality conveying meaning and affect.
  • Development of Speech Sounds:

    • By 6 months, infants can produce most of the 46 phonemes in English.
    • Early consonants: ‘p, b, t, d’ (anterior articulation).
    • Later consonants: ‘k, g’ (posterior articulation) and ‘r, l, s, th, ch, y, z’ (more complex sounds).

Speech Milestones

  • Single Words: Expected by 20 months for girls and 23 months for boys (97th centile).
  • Simple Sentences: Three- to four-word sentences by 3 years for both genders.
  • Progression:
    • 6–7 months: Syllabic babble (e.g., ba, da).
    • 12 months: Two to three words with meaning.
    • 21–24 months: Two- to three-word combinations.
  • 2–3 years: Vocabulary explosion (up to 500 words in 6 months).
  • Mean Length of Utterance (MLU): Formula: Child's age in years + 1.
    • Example: A 3-year-old typically forms sentences with an average of four morphemes.
  • Intelligibility (Rule of Fours):
    • 1-year-old: 25% understandable.
    • 2-year-old: 50% understandable.
    • 3-year-old: 75% understandable.
    • 4-year-old: ~100% understandable.

Assessment Tools

  • Communication Development Inventories (e.g., MacArthur CDI):

    • Measure emerging communication skills between 16–30 months.
    • Examples: UK SureStart Language Measures, MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory.
  • Standardized Measures:

    • Used by speech and language therapists to evaluate:
      • Speech and language development.
      • Daily life impact of communication difficulties.

Key Insights

  • Girls and first-born children often develop speech earlier and exhibit more advanced sentence use.
  • Speech development involves a complex interplay of motor skills, cognitive processes, and environmental interactions.
  • Early monitoring and standardized assessments are essential to identify and address delays effectively.

Age Speech Sounds and Processes Language Comprehension
6 months - Responds to different tones of voice
12 months - Knows their own name and the meaning of "no" along with several other words
18 months - Selects objects on verbal request, points to body parts, and follows simple commands in context
2 years Phonemes: p, b, t, d, n, m  Follows commands with two key ideas
  Processes: Omits final consonants (e.g., "tap" → "tar"), consonant harmony (e.g., "dog" → "gog"), reduplication (e.g., "bottle" → "bobo")  
2½ years Phoneme: Adds w  Identifies everyday objects by their use and enjoys simple, familiar stories
  Processes: Consonant harmony (e.g., "duck" → "guk") resolves; final consonants may still be omitted  
3 years Phonemes: s, z, f; k, g, sh, ch begin emerging  Understands longer, more complex sentences, past tense, and some simple time words
  Processes: Fronting (e.g., "car" → "tar"); reduces consonant blends (e.g., "spoon" → "poon")  
3½–4 years Phonemes: Most sounds are now heard, emerging sounds include k, g, sh, ch, j, r, y  Follows instructions with three verbal concepts
  Processes: Fronting (e.g., "cat" → "tat")  
5 years Phonemes: Most sounds correct  Understands most everyday conversations unless highly ambiguous
  Processes: Some evident, e.g., "th" → "f" ("thank you" → "fank you") or "r" → "w" ("rabbit" → "wabbit")  
5½–7 years Phonemes: Ongoing difficulties with r and th may persist Understands implied meanings, including jokes and puns

Key Observations

  • Speech Sounds:

    • Progress from simpler sounds (e.g., p, b, t) to more complex ones (e.g., r, th).
    • Typical processes (e.g., fronting, consonant harmony) resolve as speech matures.
  • Language Comprehension:

    • Evolves from recognizing simple tones and words to understanding complex sentences and implied meanings.
    • By age 7, children can grasp nuanced language such as humor.

Language Comprehension and Recognition

  • Processes Involved:
    • Auditory Processing: Handling and interpreting sound information.
    • Phonological Processing: Knowledge and manipulation of speech sounds.
    • Lexical Processing: Understanding word-level meanings.
    • Semantic Processing: Comprehension of broader meanings in context.
  • Breakdowns in Language Comprehension:
  • Specific Language Impairment (SLI) often involves:
    • Poor spoken word recognition.
    • Potential deficits in auditory processing, phonology, lexical access, or semantics.

Specific Language Impairment (SLI)

  • Debated Causes:

    • Domain-Specific Deficit: Affects grammar or linguistic structures.
    • Processing Deficit: Issues with auditory temporal processing
    • Cognitive Deficits: Reduced procedural memory or general processing capacity 
    • Likely a combination of these factors.
  • Memory Implications:

    • Expressive Language Disorders: Primarily affect verbal memory.
    • Mixed Receptive-Expressive Disorders: Affect both verbal and visual memory, including working memory.
  • Differentiation from ASD:

    • Receptive language impairments and ASD can overlap (e.g., echolalia).
    • Language regression in the second year is a key indicator of ASD.

Neural Basis of Language

  1. Developmental Progression:

    • Phonological Processing: Present in the first months of life.
    • Semantic Processing: Begins around 12 months.
    • Syntactic Processing: Matures by 30 months.
  2. Functional Abnormalities in SLI:

    • Structural and functional abnormalities in:
      • Inferior Frontal Cortex
      • Temporal Cortex
    • Reduced left asymmetry in the planum temporale.
  3. Hemisphere Specialization:

    • Left Hemisphere:
      • Syntactic and lexical-semantic processing (segmental features).
    • Right Hemisphere:
      • Prosodic accentuation and boundary marking (suprasegmental features).
    • Interhemispheric Integration:
      • Via the posterior corpus callosum for coordination between hemispheres.

Neurophysiological Evidence

  • Mismatch Negativity (MMN):

    • Reflects preconscious processing of sound discrimination.
    • Linked to impaired language development in preterm children and those with SLI.
  • Event-Related Potentials (ERPs):

    • Delayed or atypical patterns in infants with high familial risk for SLI.
  • Functional MRI (fMRI):

    • Abnormal activity in:
      • Temporal cortex.
      • Orbitofrontal dorsolateral and medial frontal cortex.

Connectivity and Brain Maturation

  • Early Connectivity:

    • Ventral pathway (temporal → inferior frontal gyrus): Present at birth.
    • Dorsal pathway (temporal → premotor cortex): Matures later, supporting complex language functions.
  • Developmental Disorders:

    • May result from disconnection between language-relevant regions, affecting functional connectivity (Perani, 2011).

Genetics of Primary Speech and Language Impairments

Primary speech and language impairments have a strong genetic basis, supported by family history and twin studies. 

Genetic Background

  1. Family and Twin Studies:

    • Family history of late talking predicts "late talking toddlers" (Zubrick et al., 2007).
    • Twin studies highlight heritability in speech and language development (Dale et al., 1998).
  2. Genetic Complexity:

    • The genetic contribution is heterogeneous and multifaceted, involving multiple linkage regions and genes.
    • Linkage studies have identified regions on chromosomes 16q and 19q (SLI Consortium, 2004; Newbury et al., 2005).

Key Genes and Variants

  1. CNTNAP2:

    • Variants in the Exon 13–15 region influence early language acquisition (age 2).
    • Associated with:
      • SLI (Specific Language Impairment).
      • Increased susceptibility to autism when combined with other risk factors.
    • Encodes a neurexin protein involved in neuronal cell adhesion.
    • Enriched expression in language-related brain circuits 
    • Heritable marker: Linked to nonsense word repetition tasks, a measure of phonological working memory.
  2. FOXP2:

    • A transcription factor that regulates CNTNAP2.
    • Mutations cause a rare monogenic form of speech and language disorder, as seen in the ‘K family’ 

Phenotypes and Chromosomal Anomalies

  1. Sex Aneuploidies:

    • Klinefelter Syndrome (XXY), Triple X Syndrome (XXX), and XYY Syndrome:
      • May present with overlapping phenotypes to primary SLI.
      • Require investigation in speech and language impairment 
  2. 22q11 Deletion Syndrome:

    • Affects up to 1/3 of children with velopharyngeal insufficiency (hyper/hypo nasal phonation).
    • Microarray analysis is recommended as a first-line test (Burnell et al., 2014; Ockeleon et al., 2014).

Clinical Implications

  1. Genetic Investigations:

    • While multifactorial polygenic transmission is not yet clinically applicable, genetic testing is important in:
      • Primary language impairments with atypical presentations.
      • Secondary language impairments caused by identifiable genetic conditions.
  2. Recommendations:

    • Microarray analysis: A first-line genetic test in children with suspected speech and language disorders, particularly those with associated structural or phonation anomalies.

Syndromes of Sex Aneuploidy and Associated Language Phenotypes

47 XYY Syndrome

  • Genetic Characteristics:
    • Primary nondisjunction of the Y chromosome.
    • ~10% are mosaics (46 XY/47 XYY).
    • Incidence: ~1 per 1,000 male live births.
  • Language Phenotype:
    • Common speech and language difficulties:
      • Speech disfluency.
      • Word-finding problems.
      • Expressive language deficits (e.g., difficulty with narratives).
      • Problems understanding complex sentence structures.
    • Auditory memory problems are frequent.
    • Reading difficulties are prevalent.
    • IQ: Slightly below the population mean but can range to superior levels.

47 XXY Syndrome (Klinefelter Syndrome Group)

  • Genetic Characteristics:
    • Surplus of X chromosomes in males.
    • Caused by meiotic disjunction during gametogenesis (ovum or sperm).
    • ~50% are maternally derived.
    • Incidence: ~1 per 750 male births.
  • Language Phenotype:
    • Specific expressive speech and language deficits:
      • Word retrieval issues.
      • Syntax problems.
      • Difficulty giving narratives.
    • Underlying auditory processing and short-term auditory memory deficits.
    • Cognitive Profile:
      • Verbal IQ is lower than performance IQ.
    • Behavioral Characteristics:
      • Often socially unresponsive.
      • Tendency to withdraw from group activities.

47 XXX Syndrome (Triple X Syndrome)

  • Genetic Characteristics:
    • Arises from primary nondisjunction of maternally or paternally derived X chromosomes.
    • Incidence: ~1 per 1,000 female births.
  • Language Phenotype:
    • Severe expressive language delay is common.
    • Poor short-term auditory memory is often observed.

Turner Syndrome (45 X and Variants)

  • Genetic Characteristics:
    • Second X chromosome is absent or abnormal.
    • Incidence: ~1 per 2,000–2,500 female births.
  • Language Phenotype:
    • Verbal skills are generally intact, but:
      • Verbal fluency may be reduced.
    • Behavioral Characteristics:
      • Challenges with peer relationships are common.

;;p

  • Common Themes Across Syndromes:
    • Expressive language difficulties (e.g., narratives, syntax, word retrieval).
    • Auditory memory and processing deficits.
  • Cognitive Profiles:
    • Verbal IQ often lower than performance IQ, except in Turner Syndrome.
  • Social Challenges:
    • Social withdrawal in 47 XXY.
    • Peer relationship difficulties in Turner Syndrome.

References

Arzimanoglou, A., O'Hare, A., Johnston, M., & Ouvrier, R. (Eds.). (2018). Aicardi's Diseases of the Nervous System in Childhood (4th ed.). Mac Keith Press.

Related Articles